The Basis for Recommending the 520 Hz Fire Alarm Signal |
![]() Issue 96: The Basis for Recommending the 520 Hz Fire Alarm SignalBy Ian Thomas and Dorothy Bruck, Centre for Environmental Safety and Risk Engineering Victoria University, Melbourne Australia
It has been shown by us and others (for example Nober et.al. Fire Journal July 1981 and Kahn Fire Technology 1984) that relatively low level sounds of various types will awaken many sleeping people, but it is obvious that to awaken most people alarm sounds should be very loud in locations where people sleep (mainly bedrooms and lounge rooms) and, at a given sound level or volume, should be noticed and acted on by as many people as possible. Previous research, confirmed by our research, has shown that different sounds at a given measured sound level (dBA) will awaken sleeping people in different proportions, depending on a variety of characteristics as well as individual differences.
The groups of sleeping people tested have included children, unimpaired adults, adults impaired with alcohol, adults impaired with hypnotics (sleeping tablets), adults aged over 65 years and adults who are hard of hearing. Other than the unimpaired adults, these are people who are unusually vulnerable to injury and death from fire (particularly while they are asleep).
Many different sounds have been used in our testing of the response of these people while sleeping. These sounds include the current smoke alarm signal used in Australia, the US and many other countries (an ~3100 Hz pure tone), pure tones of 400, 800 and 1600 Hz, 400, 520, 800 and 1600 Hz square wave sounds, white noise, whooping sounds (one varying between 400-1600 Hz and another between 400-800 Hz) and voice messages (child's mother's voice, voices of male and female actors). In all of these tests the 520 Hz square wave has been found to be as good or better at waking people than the others while the current smoke alarm sound has consistently been found to be the worst of those tested. A useful comparison of the current smoke alarm sound (~3100 Hz tone) with the 520 Hz square wave sound when presented at the same level at the pillow (75 dBA except as noted below) compares the proportion of people in the various groups who slept through the alarm. (The proportion that slept through represents the people who, because they are unresponsive, remain vulnerable to the fire even though the alarm has sounded.) The comparison uses the ratio of the proportion that did not respond to the current smoke alarm sound and the proportion that did not respond to the 520 Hz square wave sound. Thus a ratio of 1:1 means equal proportions, and a ratio of 12:1 means that 12 times as many people did not respond to the current smoke alarm sound compared with the 520 Hz square wave. In the following, N = number of people tested and BAC = Blood Alcohol Concentration. Thus of:
This comparison makes it obvious that at the same sound level (75 dBA) the 520 Hz square wave is far superior to the current alarm sound for waking all of these vulnerable people. An explanation for this superiority involves the complexity of a square wave sound. The 520 Hz square wave has been described as being a dissonant noise. It has a fundamental frequency of 520 Hz and subsequent peaks at the 3rd, 5th, 7th etc. harmonics. These multiple peaks are less likely to be masked by ambient noise than a single frequency. Several of the peaks are in the frequency range where human hearing is most sensitive, are also more than a critical bandwidth apart, and the different frequencies activate different parts of the basilar membrane in the inner ear thus increasing the perceived loudness.
More detailed information on the research and the results is available in the following references (* = review papers) *Bruck, D. and Thomas, I., (2008). Towards a better smoke alarm signal – an evidence based approach, Karlsson B. (ed.), Proceedings of the 9th International Symposium of the International Association for Fire Safety Science, Karlsruhe, Germany, pp 403-414. Bruck, D., Ball, M., Thomas, I. and Rouillard, V., (2009). How does the pitch and pattern of a signal affect auditory arousal thresholds? Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 196-203. Lykiardopoulos, C., Bruck, D. and Ball, M., (2014). The Effect of Hypnotics on Auditory Arousal Thresholds in Older Adults. 22nd Congress of European Sleep Research Society, Tallinn, Estonia, 16-20th Sept 2014 (P343). *Thomas, I. and Bruck, D. (2009). Awakening of Sleeping People – a Decade of Research Fire Technology, 46 (3), 743-761. Related Articles:
For questions concerning delivery of this eNewsletter, please contact our Customer Service Department at (216) 931-9934 or magazine.sfpe.org.
Copyright 2015, SFPE and Penton. |